You can’t put the
toothpaste back in the tube. You can’t put the genie back in the bottle. You
can’t turn back the clock. We have lots of expressions denoting the
irreversibility of certain actions. Getting patients to stop taking a
particular medication or to abandon a procedure—and persuading doctors to stop
prescribing it—seems to be in the same camp. Unless something comes along
that’s much better, or at least that the pharmaceutical or device manufacturing
company says is much better, we are extremely reluctant to change. A series of
articles in JAMA Internal Medicine
discuss this
phenomenon, calling it “the undiffusion of established practices.”
One article reports that
physicians gradually adopted the practice of tight control of blood sugars in
the ICU after a single study showed that patients did better when their blood
sugars were as close to normal as possible, but then abruptly gave up the
practice after a later study drew the opposite conclusion. A second article describes how
physicians jumped on the bandwagon of ordering noninvasive screening for coronary disease in patients
scheduled for non-cardiac surgery, only to discover later that the screening
was not beneficial—and the routine use of such testing decreased. A third article discloses that
professional medical societies tend to continue to support practices that have
been challenged or even debunked if their members believe the practices to be
useful.
What does all this
tell us about “undiffusion?” How do we
change behavior? One answer is that we should move slowly to adopt something
new, whether a medication, a procedure, or a process. We should exercise
caution, given the reality that the best new thing often turns out not to be
the best. A second answer is to design studies that are better at looking for
subgroups that may benefit from a new approach—even if the approach doesn’t
make sense for everyone. But I think there’s a lot more to be said about
putting the genie back in the bottle, particularly as we begin to recognize
that less may be more.
It’s instructive to
look at the cases where the genie did go back in the bottle and compare them to
the cases in which he didn’t. Consider, for example, the process of tightly
controlling blood sugars in the ICU, a practice that physicians readily
abandoned. What’s striking about this is that controlling blood sugars very
tightly is a nuisance—it means checking the blood sugar four times a day and
adjusting insulin doses every time. That’s labor intensive—someone has to do
the tests and someone else has to respond to the results. And there’s nothing
sexy about pricking a patient’s finger to get a drop of blood. It’s not nearly
as exciting as seeing the patient’s organs in color on a CT-scan image—a test
that was not so readily abandoned. Finally, there’s no drug company or device
manufacturer that stands to gain from widespread use of fingersticks, though
there is from diffusion of 128-slice CT scanners, not to mention 256-slice
machines.
What about the
recent example in which use of antidepressants in young people decreased after
the FDA issued a black box warning about potential side effects? Some critics argue that in this instance, putting the toothpaste back in the tube
was wrong-headed: many more people suffered from not being treated than from
the side-effects of treatment. Whether or not it was a good idea to cut back on
antidepressant use, the major driver of the behavioral change appears to have
been the media. Newspapers reported that antidepressants were bad; they were
dangerous; they killed people. They presented a far less nuanced picture than
the original article. But the combination of publicity and fear-mongering was
effective and antidepressant prescribing rates fell significantly.
Finally, the most
dramatic, if obvious, examples of turning back the clock occur when the FDA
orders a drug company to recall its drug. When Vioxx was taken off the market, after revelations that it significantly increased the risk of heart attacks, use of Vioxx plummeted. In a similar vein, when health insurance companies
decide not to pay for something, use generally falls. Bone marrow
transplantation for the treatment of metastatic breast cancer ceased when,
based on revelations that an earlier study had been fraudulent, coverage was
halted.
Going back is hard.
Commenting on the articles in JAMA Internal Medicine, Frank Davidoff quotes
David Hume: “Men generally fix their affections more on what they are possessed
of, than on what they never enjoyed. For this reason, it would be greater
cruelty to dispossess a man of any thing, than not to give it to him.” Likewise, research in cognitive psychology
confirms that people are far more distressed by what they perceive to be losses
than they are ecstatic about what they see as gains. So reversing course is likely to require a
multi-pronged approach. We need to have more solid evidence to adopt change in
the first place and we should be
looking more carefully at subgroups of the population when we draw our initial
inferences about efficacy and we need
more measured and judicious reporting by the news media and we need to use the nuclear option—removing ineffective or
harmful interventions from the market or from third party coverage. Only then
will we gain control over the phenomenon of persistent use of established but
suboptimal treatment.
No comments:
Post a Comment