The enormous interest in
getting good “value” for every dollar spent on health care, whether by
individuals, insurers, government, or anyone else neglects certain basic
realities—for example, that medical care isn’t a consumer good like toasters:
it’s a very sophisticated service provided by highly trained
professionals; and that health insurance
by its very nature makes the operation of a free market impossible. There’s
still another basic reality that is even more often neglected, and that is the
widespread belief that “you get what you pay for.” Or, if you pay less for one
treatment than another, the cheaper one is necessarily inferior. Any claims
that the two are of equal quality are suspect. And claims that the cheaper one
is higher quality are, on their face, deemed outlandish.
Translated into practice,
this means that patients and doctors alike tend to assume that more is better.
More x-rays (or, as plain radiographs, CT scans, MRIs, and PET scans are
collectively known, “imaging studies”), more medications, more doctors is superior care and must result in better outcomes. As a
result, I’m not at all surprised that changing physician behavior and patient
expectations has proved difficult, even when professional guidelines assert
that less is more. And unfortunately (unfortunate since, from a geriatric
perspective, less often is more), a
new study that purports to show that greater spending per hospitalized patient
fails to improve outcomes is hardly convincing.
Previous retrospective
studies, especially those comprising the Dartmouth Atlas of Health Care, have shown that expenditures
on apparently similar patients differ by geographic region, by hospital, and
within regions—without any measurable difference in outcomes. But the Dartmouth
Atlas has been criticized for working backwards from death even though death
could not have been predicted in advance, it has been criticized for failing to
adequately consider differences between the patient population in different locales,
and it has been critiqued for not acknowledging that patient preference might
account for some of the observed differences in health care utilization and, as
a result, in cost. The new study asks whether physicians working in the same hospital nonetheless exhibit
differences in their pattern of test- and treatment-ordering and whether that
variation results in different outcomes for their patients. Looking at over 1.3
million hospitalizations occurring at over 3000 hospitals and involving 72,000 physicians,
they found large variability in expenditures and no difference in outcomes—just
like the Dartmouth Atlas findings.
The authors were careful to
look at Medicare Part B spending because this is involves services that are at
the discretion of physicians (Part A spending is determined largely by the DRG,
the reason for admission, and is set by Medicare) and is a “proxy” for the
intensity of resource use by physicians. They were careful to confine their
analysis to Medicare fee-for-service beneficiaries who were age 65 or older and
hospitalized for an acute medical condition. And they examined separately the
behavior of general internists and hospitalists. They made some adjustments to
account for differences among patients, including age (in 5-year increments),
sex, race/ethnicity, median income, and existing comorbidities, and other
adjustments to account for differences among physicians, including age (also in
5-year increments), sex, and site of medical school education. They found that
the variation in spending across physicians within a hospital was greater than
across hospitals. Among hospitalists, adjusted spending was more than 40
percent higher among doctors in the highest spending quartile compared with the
lowest quartile. And higher expenditures had no effect on either the 30-day
readmission rate or mortality, the two measures of quality used to examine
outcomes.
Regrettably, this study has a
number of glaring weaknesses. First, there are the odd omissions: the authors
report on the gap between the highest and lowest quartiles of hospitalists but
not the corresponding figure for general internists, even though nearly twice
as many patients were cared for by internists than by hospitalists. Next, it’s
not clear that the two outcomes examined—mortality and readmission rate—are
good indicators of quality. Or rather, even if the two groups were
indistinguishable based on these two measures, perhaps one group fared far
better than the other on some other measure that wasn’t looked at, say quality
of life. Finally, the study wasn’t randomized and it wasn’t prospective,
allowing for the possibility that there were important differences between the
patients on whom much money was spent and those on whom less was spent. In
fact, maybe the patients on whom more resources were expended were sicker. If
they were sicker but had the same mortality rate and readmission rate as those
on whom fewer resources were spent, then arguably they fared better than their
counterparts!
So where do we go from here?
Contrary to the prevailing wisdom, the answer may not lie with “big data.” Too
many things are going on at once with these patients to be able to reliably
conclude that ceteris paribus, all things being equal, overall expenditure on
tests and treatments had no bearing on outcomes. I think it would make sense to
look at a small number of detailed case examples—20 or 30 patients of the same
age with the same admitting diagnosis, matched for severity of illness,
co-morbidities, race, ethnicity, and socioeconomic class, some of whom are
cared for by prolific test-orderers and some of whom are not—following them
prospectively over time to see what happens to them. And the study would try to
ascertain why various choices were made, perhaps by interviewing the patients
and/or their doctors, perhaps by gleaning the answer from free text in medical
records, and what their outcomes turned out to be.
No comments:
Post a Comment